Difference between revisions of "Quasi-Experimental Methods"
(Created page with "== Introduction == Like Experimental Methods, quasi-experimental research methods are intended to satisfy the conditions where a researcher can construct a model where ex...") |
|||
Line 2: | Line 2: | ||
Like [[Experimental Methods]], quasi-experimental research methods are intended to satisfy the conditions where a researcher can construct a model where exposure to a treatment effect is uncorrelated with other characteristics. Unlike in experimental research methods, the investigator does not have direct control over the exposure, and instead uses "natural experiments" such as [[Regression Discontinuity]] designs to identify existing circumstances where random assignment of some treatment took place. | Like [[Experimental Methods]], quasi-experimental research methods are intended to satisfy the conditions where a researcher can construct a model where exposure to a treatment effect is uncorrelated with other characteristics. Unlike in experimental research methods, the investigator does not have direct control over the exposure, and instead uses "natural experiments" such as [[Regression Discontinuity]] designs to identify existing circumstances where random assignment of some treatment took place. | ||
== The Power of Quasi-Experimental Methods == | |||
Like experimental methods, quasi-experimental methods aim to solve the problems of confoundedness and reverse or simultaneous causality in identifying the marginal effect of a change in condition. Unlike experimental methods, quasi-experiments tend to be based on policy changes or natural events that were not actually randomly assigned, but occurred in such a way that they can be considered "as good as randomly assigned" relative to some of the outcomes under consideration. Two examples illustrate these differences well. | |||
First, consider a policy shock that applies to all people participating in a given market (such as a | |||
Second, consider a natural disaster that has a various impact across space, such as an earthquake with a random fault/epicenter activation. Assuming that there was no way for households to foresee the timing or location of the earthquake, and that there was no serious differential attrition on critical dimensions, it can be used as a natural experiment on outcomes later in life for affected individuals. However, for the input component to be truly exogenous, something like the distance to the faultline should be used as the treatment variable - even though estimates of local magnitude may be available. This is because the local magnitude may be correlated with other varying characteristics (such as soil quality or hilliness) that also contribute to the outcomes (such as family income) and therefore re-introduce the confounding problem. Distance from a randomly placed disaster, though a less precise measure of "exposure", is critically uncorrelated with almost any imaginable outcome under the correct assumption (unlike, for example, distance to a volcano, or to a tsunami-affected shoreline). |
Revision as of 19:52, 30 January 2018
Introduction
Like Experimental Methods, quasi-experimental research methods are intended to satisfy the conditions where a researcher can construct a model where exposure to a treatment effect is uncorrelated with other characteristics. Unlike in experimental research methods, the investigator does not have direct control over the exposure, and instead uses "natural experiments" such as Regression Discontinuity designs to identify existing circumstances where random assignment of some treatment took place.
The Power of Quasi-Experimental Methods
Like experimental methods, quasi-experimental methods aim to solve the problems of confoundedness and reverse or simultaneous causality in identifying the marginal effect of a change in condition. Unlike experimental methods, quasi-experiments tend to be based on policy changes or natural events that were not actually randomly assigned, but occurred in such a way that they can be considered "as good as randomly assigned" relative to some of the outcomes under consideration. Two examples illustrate these differences well.
First, consider a policy shock that applies to all people participating in a given market (such as a
Second, consider a natural disaster that has a various impact across space, such as an earthquake with a random fault/epicenter activation. Assuming that there was no way for households to foresee the timing or location of the earthquake, and that there was no serious differential attrition on critical dimensions, it can be used as a natural experiment on outcomes later in life for affected individuals. However, for the input component to be truly exogenous, something like the distance to the faultline should be used as the treatment variable - even though estimates of local magnitude may be available. This is because the local magnitude may be correlated with other varying characteristics (such as soil quality or hilliness) that also contribute to the outcomes (such as family income) and therefore re-introduce the confounding problem. Distance from a randomly placed disaster, though a less precise measure of "exposure", is critically uncorrelated with almost any imaginable outcome under the correct assumption (unlike, for example, distance to a volcano, or to a tsunami-affected shoreline).