Difference between revisions of "Quasi-Experimental Methods"
Line 10: | Line 10: | ||
==Assumptions and Limitations== | ==Assumptions and Limitations== | ||
In general, quasi-experimental methods require larger samples than experimental methods. Further, for quasi-experimental methods to provide valid and unbiased estimates of program impacts, researchers must make more assumptions about the control group than in experimental methods. For example, difference-in-differences relies on the equal trends assumption (see [[Difference-in-Differences]] for more details), while matching assumes identical unobserved characteristics between the treatment and control groups. | In general, quasi-experimental methods require larger samples than experimental methods. Further, for quasi-experimental methods to provide valid and unbiased estimates of program impacts, researchers must make more assumptions about the control group than in experimental methods. For example, difference-in-differences relies on the equal trends assumption (see [[Difference-in-Differences]] for more details), while matching assumes identical unobserved characteristics between the treatment and control groups. | ||
<math>ax^2 + bx + c = 0</math> | |||
== Additional Resources == | == Additional Resources == | ||
* Robert Michael's slides on [http://www.indiana.edu/~educy520/sec6342/week_05/quasi_designs_2up.pdf Quasi-Experimental Designs] | * Robert Michael's slides on [http://www.indiana.edu/~educy520/sec6342/week_05/quasi_designs_2up.pdf Quasi-Experimental Designs] |
Revision as of 07:02, 12 February 2020
Quasi-experimental methods are research designs that that aim to identify the impact of a particular intervention, program or event (a "treatment") by comparing treated units (households, groups, villages, schools, firms, etc.) to control units. While quasi-experimental methods use a control group, they differ from experimental methods in that they do not use randomization to select the control group. Quasi-experimental methods are useful for estimating the impact of a program or event for which it is not ethically or logistically feasible to randomize. This page outlines common types of quasi-experimental methods.
Read First
- Common examples of quasi-experimental methods include difference-in-differences, regression discontinuity design, instrumental variables and matching. For more details on each type, please visit their respective pages.
- In general, quasi-experimental methods require larger sample sizes and more assumptions than experimental methods in order to provide valid and unbiased estimates of program impacts.
Overview
Like experimental methods, quasi-experimental methods aim to estimate program effects free of confoundedness, reverse causality or simultaneous causality. While quasi-experimental methods use a counterfactual, they differ from experimental methods in that they do not randomize treatment assignment. Instead, quasi-experimental methods exploit existing circumstances in which treatment assignment has a sufficient element of randomness, as in regression discontinuity design or event studies; or simulate an experimental counterfactual by constructing a control group as similar as possible to the treatment group, as in propensity score matching. Other examples of quasi-experimental methods include instrumental variables and difference-in-differences.
Assumptions and Limitations
In general, quasi-experimental methods require larger samples than experimental methods. Further, for quasi-experimental methods to provide valid and unbiased estimates of program impacts, researchers must make more assumptions about the control group than in experimental methods. For example, difference-in-differences relies on the equal trends assumption (see Difference-in-Differences for more details), while matching assumes identical unobserved characteristics between the treatment and control groups.
Additional Resources
- Robert Michael's slides on Quasi-Experimental Designs
- Gertler et al.’s Impact Evaluation in Practice