Difference between revisions of "Quasi-Experimental Methods"

Jump to: navigation, search
Line 1: Line 1:
Quasi-experimental methods are research designs that that aim to identify the impact of a particular intervention, program or event (a "treatment") by comparing treated units (households, groups, villages, schools, firms, etc.) to control units. While quasi-experimental methods use a control group, they differ from [[Experimental Methods | experimental methods]] in that they do not use [[Randomization in Stata | randomization]] to select the control group. Quasi-experimental methods are useful for estimating the impact of a program or event for which it is not morally or logistically feasible to randomize. This page outlines common types of quasi-experimental methods.
Quasi-experimental methods are research designs that that aim to identify the impact of a particular intervention, program or event (a "treatment") by comparing treated units (households, groups, villages, schools, firms, etc.) to control units. While quasi-experimental methods use a control group, they differ from [[Experimental Methods | experimental methods]] in that they do not use [[Randomization in Stata | randomization]] to select the control group. Quasi-experimental methods are useful for estimating the impact of a program or event for which it is not [[Research Ethics | ethically]] or logistically feasible to randomize. This page outlines common types of quasi-experimental methods.


==Read First==
==Read First==

Revision as of 13:59, 17 June 2019

Quasi-experimental methods are research designs that that aim to identify the impact of a particular intervention, program or event (a "treatment") by comparing treated units (households, groups, villages, schools, firms, etc.) to control units. While quasi-experimental methods use a control group, they differ from experimental methods in that they do not use randomization to select the control group. Quasi-experimental methods are useful for estimating the impact of a program or event for which it is not ethically or logistically feasible to randomize. This page outlines common types of quasi-experimental methods.

Read First

  • Common examples of quasi-experimental methods include difference-in-differences, regression discontinuity design, and matching. For more details on each type, please visit their respective pages.
  • In general, quasi-experimental methods require larger sample sizes and more assumptions than experimental methods in order to provide valid and unbiased estimates of program impacts.

Overview

Like experimental methods, quasi-experimental methods aim to estimate program effects free of confoundedness, reverse causality or simultaneous causality. While quasi-experimental methods use a counterfactual, they differ from experimental methods in that they do not randomize treatment assignment. Instead, quasi-experimental methods exploit existing circumstances in which treatment assignment has a sufficient element of randomness, as in regression discontinuity design or event studies; or simulate an experimental counterfactual by constructing a control group as similar as possible to the treatment group, as in propensity score matching. Other examples of quasi-experimental methods include difference-in-differences.

Assumptions and Limitations

In general, quasi-experimental methods require larger samples than experimental methods. Further, for quasi-experimental methods to provide valid and unbiased estimates of program impacts, researchers must make more assumptions about the control group than in experimental methods. For example, difference-in-differences relies on the equal trends assumption (see Difference-in-Differences for more details), while matching assumes identical unobserved characteristics between the treatment and control groups.

Additional Resources