Difference between revisions of "Recall Bias"

Jump to: navigation, search
Line 22: Line 22:
* Development Impact Blogpost on [http://blogs.worldbank.org/impactevaluations/decomposing-response-error-improve-consumption-survey-design Response Error in Consumption Surveys] and the related [http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/122481467999693721/pdf/WPS7646.pdf paper]
* Development Impact Blogpost on [http://blogs.worldbank.org/impactevaluations/decomposing-response-error-improve-consumption-survey-design Response Error in Consumption Surveys] and the related [http://documents.worldbank.org/curated/en/122481467999693721/pdf/WPS7646.pdf paper]
* Blog from Financial Access on [http://www.financialaccess.org/blog/2015/7/30/reliability-of-self-reported-data-recall-bias The Reliability of Self-reported Data]
* Blog from Financial Access on [http://www.financialaccess.org/blog/2015/7/30/reliability-of-self-reported-data-recall-bias The Reliability of Self-reported Data]
* Jishnu Das, Jeffrey Hammer, Carolina Sánchez-Paramo, [http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0304387811000708 The impact of recall periods on reported morbidity and health seeking behavior], In Journal of Development Economics, Volume 98, Issue 1, 2012, Pages 76-88, ISSN 0304-3878, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jdeveco.2011.07.001. Keywords: Health; Recall effects; Health seeking behavior; Impacts of sickness
* Jishnu Das, Jeffrey Hammer, Carolina Sánchez-Paramo, [http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0304387811000708 The impact of recall periods on reported morbidity and health seeking behavior], In Journal of Development Economics, Volume 98, Issue 1, 2012, Pages 76-88, ISSN 0304-3878
** Abstract: Between 2000 and 2002, we followed 1621 individuals in Delhi, India using a combination of weekly and monthly-recall health questionnaires. In 2008, we augmented these data with another 8 weeks of surveys during which households were experimentally allocated to surveys with different recall periods in the second half of the survey. We show that the length of the recall period had a large impact on reported morbidity, doctor visits; time spent sick; whether at least one day of work/school was lost due to sickness and; the reported use of self-medication. The effects are more pronounced among the poor than the rich. In one example, differential recall effects across income groups reverse the sign of the gradient between doctor visits and per-capita expenditures such that the poor use health care providers more than the rich in the weekly recall surveys but less in monthly recall surveys. We hypothesize that illnesses – especially among the poor – are no longer perceived as “extraordinary events” but have become part of “normal” life. We discuss the implications of these results for health survey methodology, and the economic interpretation of sickness in poor populations.


[[Category: Questionnaire Design]]
[[Category: Questionnaire Design]]

Revision as of 20:46, 6 November 2017

Error caused by inaccurate or incomplete recollection of events. A particular concern for retrospective survey questions.

Read First

  • Lower salience and longer recall periods increase forgetfulness [1]
  • How long is "too long" for a recall period for a survey question? It depends on the type of event: infrequent events (e.g. purchases of major assets) will be memorable for longer periods of time than routine events (e.g. use of public transportation).

Guidelines

How to avoid?

Useful strategies:

  1. Where possible, use methods to reduce recall periods for key indicators. For example, more frequent follow-up surveys by phone, or personal diaries
  2. When Piloting your Survey, carefully test different recall periods; if possible try shorter and longer periods and check for differences in variance

Subsection 2

Subsection 3

Back to Parent

This article is part of the topic Questionnaire Design


Additional Resources

  • Development Impact Blogpost on Response Error in Consumption Surveys and the related paper
  • Blog from Financial Access on The Reliability of Self-reported Data
  • Jishnu Das, Jeffrey Hammer, Carolina Sánchez-Paramo, The impact of recall periods on reported morbidity and health seeking behavior, In Journal of Development Economics, Volume 98, Issue 1, 2012, Pages 76-88, ISSN 0304-3878
    • Abstract: Between 2000 and 2002, we followed 1621 individuals in Delhi, India using a combination of weekly and monthly-recall health questionnaires. In 2008, we augmented these data with another 8 weeks of surveys during which households were experimentally allocated to surveys with different recall periods in the second half of the survey. We show that the length of the recall period had a large impact on reported morbidity, doctor visits; time spent sick; whether at least one day of work/school was lost due to sickness and; the reported use of self-medication. The effects are more pronounced among the poor than the rich. In one example, differential recall effects across income groups reverse the sign of the gradient between doctor visits and per-capita expenditures such that the poor use health care providers more than the rich in the weekly recall surveys but less in monthly recall surveys. We hypothesize that illnesses – especially among the poor – are no longer perceived as “extraordinary events” but have become part of “normal” life. We discuss the implications of these results for health survey methodology, and the economic interpretation of sickness in poor populations.